首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   874篇
  免费   19篇
教育   677篇
科学研究   29篇
各国文化   14篇
体育   57篇
文化理论   5篇
信息传播   111篇
  2023年   6篇
  2022年   8篇
  2021年   6篇
  2020年   30篇
  2019年   33篇
  2018年   45篇
  2017年   50篇
  2016年   41篇
  2015年   23篇
  2014年   22篇
  2013年   191篇
  2012年   26篇
  2011年   34篇
  2010年   21篇
  2009年   24篇
  2008年   20篇
  2007年   10篇
  2006年   16篇
  2005年   14篇
  2004年   16篇
  2003年   10篇
  2002年   15篇
  2001年   13篇
  2000年   12篇
  1999年   9篇
  1998年   4篇
  1997年   5篇
  1996年   17篇
  1995年   21篇
  1994年   15篇
  1993年   18篇
  1992年   14篇
  1991年   12篇
  1990年   4篇
  1989年   12篇
  1988年   7篇
  1987年   4篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   7篇
  1983年   5篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   5篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   9篇
  1977年   3篇
  1976年   4篇
  1973年   3篇
  1972年   3篇
  1970年   3篇
  1866年   2篇
排序方式: 共有893条查询结果,搜索用时 156 毫秒
81.
Summary Science education in New Zealand continues to have a strong research base and now development work has begun that is modelled on a view of how people learn and which is supported by the curriculum development of the wider Ministerial Review.  相似文献   
82.
83.
Presenting narrative text in computer-based instruction (CBI) is more difficult than in print lessons due to limited screen resolution and display area. The present research aimed at achieving a more compatible match between lesson content and the attributes of the presentation media. Subjects received either a print or CBI statistics lesson containing low-density (concise) narrative text, high-density (conventional) text, or the density type they preferred (learner-control). Results showed the low-density lesson to reduce completion time relative to the high-density version, while yielding equivalent achievement. Subjects receiving the lesson in the print mode had shorter completion times and higher achievement than their CBI counterparts. Attitude results showed that CBI was favorably regarded, but perceived as longer and slow-moving than print. Implications of results for designing instructional material in accord with CBI attributes and learner characteristics are discussed.  相似文献   
84.
The Office for Standards in Education (OFSTED) continues to judge schools in England and Wales to be generally unsuccessful in providing opportunities to pupils and students for spiritual development. This article reports on a study of OFSTED inspection reports for Norfolk state secondary schools. The study aimed to understand what was going wrong with spiritual development and how things might be improved. The study noted OFSTED's guidance to its inspectors on spiritual development, but then audited inspectors' assessments of spiritual development as they saw it in the field. After a close analysis of the reports the study concluded that inconsistencies in inspectors' reporting, particularly of spirituality across the broader curriculum, meant that both the mechanisms for assessment and for raising standards were not clear. The article discusses possible reasons for that lack of clarity and suggests that increased dialogue between inspectors and teachers might help raise standards in this area.  相似文献   
85.
Since the passage of PL 94‐142, nearly every professional in public education is required to enrol in a course of educating exceptional children and youth. This paper is addressed to three audiences: 1) the instructors of such courses; 2) individuals responsible for in‐service training; and 3) teachers and parents of exceptional children and youth. In this brief article, a sixty‐minute group exercise designed to demonstrate the deleterious effects of overprotection is described.  相似文献   
86.
87.
The relations between changes in the scalp-recorded electroencephalogram (EEG) and the development of the ability to perform successfully 2 cognitive tasks attributed to frontal lobe functioning were examined in 13 infants from 7 to 12 months of age. Infants successful in performing the A-not-B task with increasingly longer delays across the second half of the first year of life showed changes in power in scalp-recorded brain electrical activity in the frontal region and an increase in anterior/posterior EEG coherence. Infants with rapid mastery of object retrieval did not differ in frontal EEG development from infants who exhibited the normal developmental progression in object retrieval performance. In a task examining inhibition of reaching to a novel toy, there were no differences in frontal EEG as a function of performance. Results from a cross-sectional sample revealed similar findings. These data confirm work with nonhuman primates on the importance of maturation of frontal cortex in the successful performance on certain tasks (A-not-B), but do not confirm nonhuman primate data on the importance of frontal cortex for other tasks (object retrieval). The data also suggest that the electroencephalogram may be useful as a noninvasive measure of central nervous system development during the first year of life.  相似文献   
88.
Genetic constructivism regards the development of cognitive structures as the prod-uct of interactions among three systems of equilibration relating to phylogenesis, sociogenesis and psychogenesis (Cellérier & Decret, 1992). From this point of view, the study of numbers is of particular interest since it reveals visible interactions between the child, his current forms of cognitive organization and his particular sociocultural environments. It is also worthy of note because the major constructivist theories (the genetic psychology of Piaget, the ‘constructive’ innateness of Gelman and the sociogenesis of Fuson) have taken an interest in numbers, focusing their attention in varying degrees on one of the systems in question. Our purpose here is to show the relevance of the complementarity of these approaches to research in and the teaching of elementary arithmetic.  相似文献   
89.
Seventy‐four students read passages from an individually administered test of reading comprehension (a subtest from the Test of Dyslexia, a test of reading and related abilities currently in development; McCallum & Bell, 2001), and then answered literal and inferential questions. Students were randomly assigned to one of two conditions; 39 students read the passages silently and 35 read orally, with time recorded for each passage read. Comprehension and time were dependent measures for a Multivariate Analysis of Covariance (MANCOVA) and two follow‐up Analyses of Covariance (ANCOVA). After controlling for reading ability, results from the MANCOVA showed a significant combined effect ( p < .05); however, a comparison of mean reading comprehension scores showed no significant difference between silent readers and oral readers ( p > .05). On the other hand, with reading ability controlled, silent readers took significantly less time to complete passages compared to those who read orally ( p < .02). In fact, students took 30% longer to read orally than silently, on average. When test directions do not specify either oral or silent reading and error analysis is not a goal, testing will be more efficient via silent responding with no loss of comprehension. © 2004 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Psychol Schs 41: 241–246, 2004.  相似文献   
90.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号